Sixteen Experimental Investigations from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory - Hugo Münsterberg (best life changing books txt) 📗
- Author: Hugo Münsterberg
- Performer: -
Book online «Sixteen Experimental Investigations from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory - Hugo Münsterberg (best life changing books txt) 📗». Author Hugo Münsterberg
consciousness together. At the foot of the columns are shown the
averages for each pair. No general averages are shown, as the problem
presented by each pair is peculiar to itself.
[Illustration: FIG. 7.]
The maximum is reached in No. 1a, where the angle has the arrowhead
form and each angle points to the other. It should be remarked that
the diagram is somewhat misleading in respect to the distance of the
figures, which in this as in the other experiments was 25 cm. The
figures therefore were far enough away from each other to be perceived
and imaged in individual distinctness. But the ‘energy’ of the lines,
especially where the lines united to form an acute angle, was often
sufficient to overcome the effect of this separation, and either to
bring the figures nearer together or to unite them into a single
object. The notes are very decisive in this regard. A few of them may
be cited: “The angles tended to join points.” “The figures showed a
tendency to move in the direction of the apex.” “The angles (2a)
united to form a cross.” “When both figures (4b) were in mind I felt
disagreeable strains in the eyeballs; one figure led me to the right
and the other to the left.” The effect of the last-named figures
(4a) seemed to be different from that of 1a and 2a, though the
apex of each angle was turned to that of the other in each of the
three cases. “The two angles,” says another subject, speaking of 4a,
“appeared antagonistic to each other.” It will be observed that they
are less acute than the other angles referred to, and the confluent
lines of each figure are far less distinctly directed towards the
corresponding lines of the opposing figure, so that the attention, so
far as it is determined in direction by the lines, would be less
likely to be carried over from the one image to the other.
On the other hand, when the angles were turned away from each other
the legs of the angles in the two figures compared were brought into
closer relation, so that in 2b, for instance, the average is even
higher than in 2a. Similarly the average in 3b, an obtuse angle,
is higher than in 3a. The notes show that in such cases the
contrasted angles tended to close up and coalesce into a single
figure with a continuous boundary. “The ends (2b) came together and
formed a diamond.” “When the angles were turned away from each other
the lines had an occasional tendency to close up.” “There was a
tendency to unite the two images (4a) into a triangle.” “The two
figures seemed to tug each other, and the images were in fact a little
closer than the objects (4a).” “The images (4a) formed a
triangle.” So with regard to the figures in 5a. “When both were in
the field there seemed to be a pulling of the left over to the right,
though no apparent displacement.” “The two figures formed a square.”
The lowest average—and it is much lower than any other average in the
table—is that of 5b, in which the contrasted objects have neither
angles nor incomplete lines directed to any common point between the
objects. In view of the notes, the tabulated record of these two
figures (5b) is very significant, and strikingly confirms, by its
negative testimony, what 1a and 2b have to teach us by their
positive testimony. The averages are, in the three cases just cited:
1a, 35.11 seconds; 2b,33.11 seconds; 5b, 15 seconds per minute.
On the whole, then, the power of the line to arrest, direct, and keep
the attention, through the greater energy and definiteness of the
processes which it excites, and thereby to increase the chances of the
recurrence and persistence of its idea in consciousness, is confirmed
by the results of this series. The greatest directive force seems to
lie in the sharply acute angle. Two such angles, pointing one towards
the other, tend very strongly to carry the attention across the gap
which separates them. (And it should be borne in mind that the
distance between the objects exposed was 25 cm.) But the power of two
incomplete lines, similarly situated, is not greatly inferior.
It thus appears that the attention process is in part, at least, a
motor process, which in this case follows the direction of the lines,
acquiring thereby a momentum which is not at once arrested by a break
in the line, but is readily diverted by a change in the direction of
the line. If the lines are so situated that the attention process
excited by the one set is carried away from the other set, the one set
inhibits the other. If, on the other hand, the lines in the one set
are so situated that they can readily take up the overrunning or
unarrested processes excited by the other set, the two figures support
each other by becoming in fact one figure. The great importance of the
motor elements of the attention process in ideation, and thus in the
persistence of the idea, is evident in either phase of the experiment.
RECAPITULATION.
Seconds Seconds.
1 Figures alike: Left 30.8 Right 31.9
2 ” unlike: Simple 27.10 Complex 34.62
3 ” ” Small 24.54 Large 33.30
4 ” ” Gray 25.61 White 29.53
5 ” ” Line 31.91 Angle 38.54
6 ” ” Plain 23.92 Marked 37.48
7 ” ” (colored) 5 seconds 27.75 10 seconds 29.15
8 ” ” (gray) 5 seconds 25.42 10 ” 32.12
9 ” ” 1st exposure 12.64 2d exposure 36.45
10 ” ” Vertical line 34.94 Hor. line 34.49
11 ” ” Full-faced 28.10 Outline 41.08
12 ” ” Figure 29.26 Int. lines 39.32
13 ” ” Figure 34.03 Vert. lines 36.40
14 ” ” Stationary 28.88 Moved 37.39
15 ” ” Gray 30.90 Colored 37.81
16 (See Table XVI.)
If we put these results into the form of propositions, we find:
1. That when the objects are similar surfaces, seen under similar
conditions, the chances of the recurrence and persistence of their
images are, on the whole, practically equal.
2. That surfaces bounded by complicated outlines have an advantage in
ideation, other things equal, over surfaces bounded by simple
outlines.
3. That as between two objects of unequal area—color, form, and other
conditions being the same—the larger object has the advantage in the
ideational rivalry.
4. That the image of a white object has a like advantage over the
image of a gray object.
5. That broken or complex lines have in ideation an advantage over
straight or simple lines.
6. That an object with varied content, other conditions remaining the
same, has an advantage over an object with homogeneous surface.
7 and 8. That an increase of the time during which the attention is
given to an object increases the chances for the recurrence of its
image or idea.
9. That of two objects to which attention is directed in succession,
the object last seen has a distinct advantage in the course of
ideation following close on the perception of the objects.
10. That lines of similar appearance and equal length, one of which is
vertical and the other horizontal, have, like surfaces of similar
appearance and form and equal dimensions, practically equal chances of
recurrence and survival in ideation, the slight difference in their
chances being in favor of the vertical line.
11. That as between two figures of similar form and equal dimensions,
one of which has a filled homogeneous content and the other is a mere
outline figure, the latter has a marked advantage in the course of
ideation.
12. That of two linear and symmetrical figures, of which one is an
outline figure with continuous boundary, and the other consists of the
same linear elements, similarly disposed, as the first, but has its
lines disconnected so that it has no continuous boundary, the latter
figure has the advantage in ideation.
13. That if, with material similar to that described in paragraph 12,
the disconnected lines are arranged so as to be vertical and
equidistant, the advantage in ideation still remains with the
disconnected lines, but is much reduced.
14. That if one of two figures, of similar appearance and form and of
equal dimensions, is kept in motion while it is exposed to view, and
the other is left at rest, the image of the moving object is the more
persistent.
15. That, under like conditions, colored objects are more persistent
in ideation than gray objects.
16. That lines and sharp angles, as compared with broad surfaces, have
a strong directive force in the determination of the attention to
their images or ideas; that this directive force is strongest in the
case of very acute angles, the attention being carried forward in the
direction indicated by the apex of the angle; but that uncompleted
lines, especially when two such lines are directed towards each
other, have a similar and not much inferior force in the control of
the course of ideation.
If we should seek now to generalize these experimental results, they
would take some such form as the following:
Abstraction made of all volitional aims and all æsthetic or affective
bias, the tendency of an object to recur and persist in idea depends
(within the limits imposed by the conditions of these experiments)
upon the extent of its surface, the complexity of its form, the
diversity of its contents, the length and recency of the time during
which it occupies the attention, the definiteness of the direction
which it imparts to the attention (as in the case of angles and
lines), its state of motion or of rest, and, finally, its brightness
and its color.
These conditions, however, are for the most part but conditions which
determine the energy, diversity, complexity and definiteness of the
active processes involved in the bestowal of attention upon its
object, and the experiments show that such active processes are as
essential in ideation as in perception. The stability of an image, or
internal sensation, thus depends on the activity of its motor
accompaniments or conditions. And as the presence of an image to the
exclusion of a rival, which but for the effect of these motor
advantages would have as strong a claim as itself to the occupation of
consciousness (cf. Series I., X.), may be treated as a case of
inhibition, the greater the relative persistence of an image or idea
the greater we may say is the ‘force’ with which it inhibits its
rival. Exclusive possession of the field involves, to the extent to
which such possession is made good, actual exclusion of the rival; and
exclusion is inhibition. Our generalization, accordingly, may take the
following form:—
The inhibitory effect of an idea, apart from volitional or emotional
bias, depends upon the energy, diversity, complexity and definiteness
of the motor conditions of the idea.
*
CONTROL OF THE MEMORY IMAGE.
BY CHARLES S. MOORE.
Since Gallon’s classic investigation in the field of mental imagery
several similar investigations have been pursued in the same
direction, chiefly, however, for the purpose of discovering and
classifying types of imagination.
Little has been done in the line of developing and studying the
problems of the memory image proper, and still less, in fact almost
nothing, is to be found bearing on the control of the visual memory
image. The general fact of this control has been presented, with
greater or less detail, based upon returns from questionaries.
Comments (0)