Sixteen Experimental Investigations from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory - Hugo Münsterberg (best life changing books txt) 📗
- Author: Hugo Münsterberg
- Performer: -
Book online «Sixteen Experimental Investigations from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory - Hugo Münsterberg (best life changing books txt) 📗». Author Hugo Münsterberg
mean variation of the latter is greater in three cases out of five
than that of the former, a relation which apparently contradicts what
has already been said concerning the sensitiveness to variations which
marks the intervals separating rhythmical groups. The reason for this
final increase in variation appears when the relative intensities of
the series of reactions are considered. They are given in Table LXXX.
TABLE LXXX.
No. of Beats. Acc. Beat. Av. Unacc. Final. Pre-final.
Five, 1.000 0.543 0.518 0.500
Six, 1.000 0.623 0.608 0.592
Seven, 1.000 0.515 0.544 0.437
Eight, 1.000 0.929 0.949 0.863
Ten, 1.000 0.621 0.640 0.545
In every case the final element is marked by an increase over that
which precedes it (see last two columns of table) of the average value
for all rhythms of 1.000:0.900; an increase which raises it above the
average value of the whole series of preceding unaccented beats in
three cases out of five. To this final accentuation the increase in
variation is to be attributed. Yet despite the additional element of
disturbance due to this increased final stress the average value of
the mean variation for this final interval is lower than that of the
median unaccented intervals in the ratio (all rhythms combined) of
0.992:1.000.
Turning, then, to Table LXXIX., there is presented, firstly, an excess
of variation in the accented element over that of the average
unaccented elements in every case but one (the six-beat rhythm in
which the values are nearly identical), which for the whole series of
rhythms has a value of 1.000:0.794. Secondly, in every completed case
(part of the figures in the last rhythm are inadvertently lacking),
the average mean variation of the single interval preponderates over
that of the total group.
The second form of rhythmical tapping, in which the longer series were
beaten out as pairs of equal subgroups, was added in order to
determine the quantitative relations of the mean variations for
alternate subgroups when such groups were purposely intended, instead
of appearing in the form of unconscious modifications of the
rhythmical treatment, as heretofore. At the same time the results
present an additional set of figures embodying the relations here in
question. They are as follows:
TABLE LXXXI.
Intervals. Groups.
Number Av. 1st 2d 1st 2d
of Beats. Acc. Unacc. Half. Half. Half. Half. Average Totals
Six, 27.9% 20.9% 23.4% 23.0% 14.6% 13.3% 13.9% 13.8%
Eight, 16.6 14.8 13.2 17.3 6.2 3.3 4.7 2.7
Ten, 7.9 2.6 3.4 4.0 5.9 5.2 5.5 3.1
No exception here occurs to the characteristic predominance in
instability of the accented element. As regards simple intervals, the
relation of first and second groups is reversed, the reason for which
I do not know. It may be connected with the rapid speed at which the
series of reactions was made, and its consequent raising of the
threshold of perceptible variation, proportional to the value of the
whole interval, to which is also due the higher absolute value of the
variations which appear in both tables.
These inversions disappear when we compare the relative stability of
the first and second subgroups, in which the excess of variation in
the former over the latter is not only constant but great, presenting
the ratio for all three rhythms of 1.000:0.816. The characteristic
relation of lower to higher rhythmical syntheses also is here
preserved in regard to the two subgroups and the total which they
compose.
The points here determined are but a few of the problems regarding the
structure of larger rhythmical sequences which are pressing for
examination. Of those proximate to the matter here under
consideration, the material for an analysis of the mean variation in
intensity of a series of rhythmical reactions is contained in the
measurements taken in the course of the present work, and this may at
a future time be presented. The temporal variations having once been
established it becomes a minor point.
Such conclusions, however, are only preliminary to an investigation of
the characteristic structure of the ordinary metrical forms, and to
these attention should next be turned. The configuration of the common
meters should be worked out both in relation to the whole formal
sequence, and to the occurrence within the series of characteristic
variations. There can be no question that each metrical structure, the
iambic trimeter or dactylic tetrameter line, for example, composes a
definite rhythmical melody within which each measure is shortened or
prolonged, subdued or emphasized, according to its position and
connections in the series of relations which constitute the rhythmical
sequence.
These several metrical forms should be explored and the characters of
each measure in the series quantitatively determined. Such an
investigation would include an ascertainment of the proportional
time-value of each successive measure, its average force, and its
sensitiveness to variations, temporal and intensive. It should include
an examination of the configuration of the single measure and the
changes in distribution of accents and intervals which it undergoes as
the rhythmical series advances. For the rhythm group must not be
conceived as a simple unchanging form; both intensively and temporally
it is moulded by its function in the whole sequence, the earlier
iambic of a heroic measure being unlike the later, the dactyl which
precedes a measure of finality different from that which introduces
the series. Such a set of determinations will give the pure
characteristic curves of our common poetical meters.
But these meters are no more simple forms than are their constituent
measures. At every point their structure is subject to modification by
factors which appear in the rhythmic utterance in virtue of its use as
a medium for the free expression of thought and emotion; and the
manner in which the characteristic form is altered by these factors of
variation must be studied. Of these variations the more important are
the effects of the introduction of variants—of spondees among
dactyls, of anapæsts among iambics, and the like—and the occurrence
of points of origin, emphasis, interruption, and finality in special
accentuations, syncopated measures, cæsural pauses and elisions. These
factors influence the structure both of those measures within which
they appear and of those adjacent to them. The nature and extent of
this wave of disturbance and its relation to the configuration of the
whole sequence call for examination.
Finally, this process of investigation should be applied to the larger
structures of the couplet and stanza, that the characteristic
differences in the pair or series of verses involved may be
determined. These characters include the whole time occupied by each
verse of the stanza, the relative values of acatalectic and catalectic
verses occurring within the same stanza structure, differences in
rhythmical melody between the latter forms, the variations of average
intensity in the accentual elements of such lines, and a determination
of the values of rests of higher and lower degrees—mid-line, verse,
and couplet pauses—which appear in the various stanza forms, and
their relation to other structural elements.
*
RHYTHM AND RHYME.
BY R.H. STETSON.
I. INTRODUCTION.
The psychological theory of rhythm has its beginnings in the work of
Herbart,[1] who inaugurated the treatment of rhythm as a species of
time perception and suggested an explanation of its emotional effects.
While Herbart had simply pointed out the effect of a whole rhythmic
series in giving rise to an emotion of expectation, delay, or haste,
Lotze[2] applied the principle severally to each unit group (each
foot) in the rhythm, and made the emotional effect of rhythm depend on
these alternate feelings of strain, expectation, and satisfaction
produced by every repetition of the unit group. Vierordt[3] did the
first experimental work on rhythm, determining the period of greatest
regularity in the tapping of rhythms. But the first important
experiments were carried on by von Brücke.[4] By tapping out rhythms
on a kymograph, he determined the well-known ‘Taktgleichheit’ of the
feet in scanned verse, and noted a number of facts about the time
relations of the different unit groups. Mach[5] added to the previous
knowledge about rhythm certain observations on the subjective
accentuation of an objectively uniform series, and specially he noted
that the process is involuntary. With a much clearer understanding of
the facts of rhythm than his predecessors had had, he really provided
the foundation for the theories which follow. His most important
contribution, for some time overlooked, was his emphasis of the
essentially motor nature of the phenomena of rhythm, and his motor
theory therefor.
[1] Herbart, J.F.: ‘Psychol. Untersuchungen’ (Sämmt. Werk,
herausgeg. von Hartenstein), Leipzig, 1850-2, Bd. VII., S. 291
ff.
[2] Lotze, R.H.: ‘Geschichte der Æsthetik,’ München, 1863, S.
487 ff.
[3] Vierordt, K.: ‘Untersuchungen über d. Zeitsinn,’ Tübingen,
1868.[4] von Brücke, E.W.: ‘Die physiol. Grundlagen d.
neuhochdeutschen Verskunst,’ Wien, 1871.
[5] Mach, Ernst: ‘Unters. ü. d. Zeitsinn d. Ohres,’ _Wiener
Sitz. Ber., mathem. naturw. Classe_, 1865, Bd. 51, II., S. 133.
Beiträge zur Physiol. d. Sinnesorgane, S. 104 ff.
Many of the recent theories of rhythm are based on Wundt’s analysis.
The work of Wundt and Dietze,[6] was concerned with rhythmic series;
but it may be noted that the ‘span of consciousness’ and the
‘synthetic activity of consciousness’ were the subjects actually under
investigation. Rhythm was considered as a special temporal form of
this ‘psychic synthesis.’ There are three different elements in a
sound series, declared these writers, which contribute to this
synthesis: qualitative changes, intensive changes and melodic changes.
Of these the intensive changes are the most important. Every increase
in intensity, that is, every beat (‘Hebung’) is followed by a
decrease, and the next increase which follows is recognized as a
repetition of the preceding beat and as the forerunner of the beat
which is to follow. From this comes the synthetic power of the rhythm.
Just as the simple unit groups are built up by this synthesizing
power, so they in turn are combined into larger phrases and periods.
The motor factor has little place in Wundt’s own discussion,[7] the
‘mental activity’ is the all-important thing. Bolton[8] also made a
very important contribution to the experimental knowledge of rhythm.
His work was based entirely on Wundt’s theory. His method of
experimentation was accurate and his observations copious. The
arrangement of his apparatus, however, led him to emphasize objective
uniformity as a condition of rhythmic grouping; so that Meumann’s
criticism of his application of this principle to poetry is quite
just. Nevertheless Bolton established the essential facts of
subjective accentuation and apparent temporal displacement. It is
noteworthy that he laid great emphasis on the motor aspect of rhythm,
and made many careful observations on the ‘motor accompaniment.’ While
inclining strongly to a motor interpretation he did not attempt to cut
loose from the Wundtian ‘apperceptive process’ as the primary factor.
[6] Wundt, W.: ‘Physiol. Psych.,’ 4te Aufl., Leipzig, 1893, Bd.
II., S. 83.
[7] Wundt, W.: ‘Physiol. Psych.,’ 4te Aufl., Leipzig, 1893,
II., S. 89 ff.
[8] Bolton, T.L.: Amer. Jour. of Psych., 1894, VI., p. 145 et
seq.
The most elaborate consideration of rhythm yet published is that of
Meumann.[9] He avowedly worked out and defended the theory of Wundt.
The only important difference is the larger place which he gave to the
‘motor accompaniment,’ although he was always careful to emphasize its
secondary and derived character. He insisted that the ‘mental
activity’ is always primary, and that without it there can be no
rhythmization; and he opposed vigorously the motor inclinations of
Mach and Bolton. It is certainly unfortunate that rhythm has always
fallen into the hands of the investigators of the ‘attention,’ or the
‘span of consciousness,’ or the ‘perception of time.’ It is but an
incident that judgments of time are often based on rhythms; and
everything that Meumann has said of a ‘mental prius,’ or
Comments (0)